Kate Elwood discusses the cultural aspects of giving and receivng compliments in her column Cultural Conundrums:
Mark Twain famously declared, "I can live for two months on a good
compliment." But for those with heartier accolade appetites, an
"emergency compliment" website was launched last October by Megs Senk,
an art director at Weiden+Kennedy, an American advertising agency. When
in urgent need of a commendation pick-me-up, praise cravers need only
turn to the screen for offbeat random admiration, like "Eight out of 10
coworkers agree your desk is the cleanest" or "You're the best at making
cereal." The playful site has already garnered more than 2 million
visitors. Of course, the notion of on-the-spot cyber-approbation has
attracted its share of derisively eye-rolling detractors as well.
Compliments in usual face-to-face interaction are notoriously fiddly
and even more so in cross-cultural contexts. Who can you compliment and
what can you say? How should you respond when on the receiving end?
Communication researcher Hiroko Matsuura took a survey of Japanese and
American students' feelings about complimenting. The responses were
similar between the two groups for many items, particularly those
related to praise motivation, which both groups agreed was to make the
other person happy or as a good way to start a conversation and not in
order to get a compliment in return.
There was, however, some noticeable divergence regarding a few
questions related to compliment-bestowing as a cultural custom.
Americans generally agreed that it was easy for them to make small
compliments, but their Japanese counterparts disagreed. Americans also
agreed that complimenting was an important part of their culture but the
Japanese respondents somewhat disagreed. When questioned about the
likelihood of complimenting in specific situations, Matsuura also found
that the American students were more likely to compliment family members
than the Japanese students.
Perhaps the true beauty of the online compliment generator is that
it eliminates the need to respond. The online user can simply bask in
the nutty glory of the moment. On the other hand, being on the receiving
end of a face-to-face friendly comment often requires the recipient to
struggle to find the right tone--not too boastful, not too
dismissive--and in cross-cultural situations frequently failing.
Applied linguist Takafumi Shimizu made a study of compliment
responses of Japanese people speaking in Japanese, Americans speaking in
English, Americans studying Japanese in Japan, and Americans studying
Japanese in America. Shimizu broke down the responses into three basic
categories: 1) positive, such as thanking, agreeing, or expressing
gladness; 2) avoidance, like changing the subject or making some
explanation; or 3) negative, for example, disagreeing or expressing
embarrassment.
Forty-eight percent of the native Japanese speakers made positive
responses compared with 81 percent of the Americans speaking English,
with the two groups of learners of Japanese falling midway between these
two groups. However, when it came to frequency of avoidance strategies,
the learners of Japanese in Japan actually had higher frequencies than
the native Japanese speakers. Thirty-nine percent of the Japanese native
speakers avoided responding to compliments, but 45 percent of the
Japanese learners in Japan did. Only 15 percent of the Americans
speaking English and 18 percent of the Japanese learners in the United
States made use of this strategy.
When making negative responses to compliments, learners of Japanese
really outdid themselves, with much higher frequencies than either the
native Japanese or native English speakers. Only 13 percent of the
native Japanese speakers and 4 percent of the Americans speaking English
disagreed with the compliments, expressed embarrassment, and so on, but
34 percent of the Japanese learners in Japan and 58 percent of the
Japanese learners in the United States did.
In addition, when the Japanese learners disagreed they did so using
stronger language than the Japanese native speakers. The Japanese native
speakers only used words of direct negation like iya, iie, or ie ie in
six out of a total of 240 responses; but the Japanese learners in Japan
used these words 28 out of 192 times; and the Japanese learners in the
United States did so in 64 out of 192 times. Moreover, the Japanese
learners often used blunt-sounding phrases like chigau ("That's wrong")
or so ja nai ("That's not right"), while the Japanese native speakers
typically used the softer sonna koto nai ("It's not really like that").
Shimizu's results lend further support to the argument for the
importance of eschewing simple but ultimately misleading
overgeneralizations in cultural communication patterns. Victor Hugo
compared compliments to a "kiss through a veil." When that veil is a
cultural shroud, care must be taken lest the kiss be met inadvertently
with a kick in the teeth.
Recent Comments